Skip to main content

Thinking Style and Threat-related Processing in Paranoia

The Psychosis Research Partnership
We, Professor Philippa Garety, Professor Elizabeth Kuipers, Dr Helen Waller and Dr Amy Hardy, are Research Clinical Psychologists based at the Department of Psychology, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London. 
We work with the Psychosis Research Partnership (Professor David Fowler, Professor Paul Bebbington, Professor Daniel Freeman, Dr Richard Emsley & Professor Graham Dunn) on a research programme funded by the Maudsley Charity and the Wellcome Trust. This research has found it is common for people to have thoughts about others intending to cause them harm, which do not seem to be a valid reflection of the shared reality of others. These can range from fleeting ideas that someone on the street might be laughing at us, to more elaborate and persistent beliefs such as that the secret services are trying to have us killed. Most people experience paranoid thoughts occasionally, but for some the preoccupation, distress and conviction related to paranoia increases and it can become a defining part of their life.


Our research group is interested in understanding the psychological factors that contribute to the development and maintenance of these paranoid thoughts, guided by cognitive-behavioural models for emotional disorders. This perspective argues that an individual’s interpretation of an event, rather than the event per se, influences what they think, feel and do in response. In turn, their interpretation is affected by a range of factors: their life history; thinking styles; unusual sensory-perceptual experiences; attentional processes; emotional states, and beliefs about the self, the world, and other people.

The research we have conducted suggests two factors are particularly key in influencing paranoid thoughts. First, the way in which people think. This includes our ability to consider different explanations for events other than a paranoid interpretation, and what and how much information we gather in deciding how to interpret an event. Second, the extent to which we engage in threat-related processing, such as feeling anxious, thinking negatively about ourselves, worrying, being biased to paying attention to danger, believing others are risky, and doing things to protect ourselves. 

When our thoughts, feelings and actions are focused on threat, we are more likely to be paranoid and believe others will cause us harm. For example, Sam believes that people in her area think she is a bad person, and that everywhere she goes people watch and gossip about her. This started after she was made redundant from her job when her company made cutbacks. She thought from the start she would be the one to go because people did not like her, and now thinks that everyone has found out about her being rejected. 

Sam always found it difficult to fit in at work and school, as she often feels anxious and struggles to talk to other people. Sam’s dad used to criticise her, telling her she was “useless” and “no good”, and she was bullied by other children when growing up. Now, when Sam goes out, she feels very on edge, and straight away notices people looking at and talking about her. This makes her feel even more anxious and worried, and so she keeps to herself. She tries not to go out unless she has to, even though she wants to have friends and get back to work. 

Identifying the key processes that contribute to paranoia has helped our team to develop talking treatments to help people like Sam manage the impact it has on their lives. These therapies target the reasoning styles and anxiety processes that we know are critical to paranoid thinking, and we have shown that they can help people feel less distressed and cope better with paranoia. 

An important challenge for cognitive-behavioural therapies for paranoid thinking is to find ways of helping people that they find interesting and enjoyable to do, and are cost effective for mental health services. We believe that new technologies present exciting opportunities for delivering these interventions, and we are currently investigating the feasibility of two brief, computerised interventions (“The Insider’s Guide to Thinking Well” and “Street Smart”) that target how people think about paranoid thoughts and cope with anxiety, to help them manage their fears about others.

Popular posts from this blog

Delusions in the DSM 5

This post is by Lisa Bortolotti. How has the definition of delusions changed in the DSM 5? Here are some first impressions. In the DSM-IV (Glossary) delusions were defined as follows: Delusion. A false belief based on incorrect inference about external reality that is firmly sustained despite what almost everyone else believes and despite what constitutes incontrovertible and obvious proof or evidence to the contrary. The belief is not one ordinarily accepted by other members of the person's culture or subculture (e.g., it is not an article of religious faith). When a false belief involves a value judgment, it is regarded as a delusion only when the judgment is so extreme as to defy credibility.

Rationalization: Why your intelligence, vigilance and expertise probably don't protect you

Today's post is by Jonathan Ellis , Associate Professor of Philosophy and Director of the Center for Public Philosophy at the University of California, Santa Cruz, and Eric Schwitzgebel , Professor of Philosophy at the University of California, Riverside. This is the first in a two-part contribution on their paper "Rationalization in Moral and Philosophical thought" in Moral Inferences , eds. J. F. Bonnefon and B. Trémolière (Psychology Press, 2017). We’ve all been there. You’re arguing with someone – about politics, or a policy at work, or about whose turn it is to do the dishes – and they keep finding all kinds of self-serving justifications for their view. When one of their arguments is defeated, rather than rethinking their position they just leap to another argument, then maybe another. They’re rationalizing –coming up with convenient defenses for what they want to believe, rather than responding even-handedly to the points you're making. Yo...

A co-citation analysis of cross-disciplinarity in the empirically-informed philosophy of mind

Today's post is by  Karen Yan (National Yang Ming Chiao Tung University) on her recent paper (co-authored with Chuan-Ya Liao), " A co-citation analysis of cross-disciplinarity in the empirically-informed philosophy of mind " ( Synthese 2023). Karen Yan What drives us to write this paper is our curiosity about what it means when philosophers of mind claim their works are informed by empirical evidence and how to assess this quality of empirically-informedness. Building on Knobe’s (2015) quantitative metaphilosophical analyses of empirically-informed philosophy of mind (EIPM), we investigated further how empirically-informed philosophers rely on empirical research and what metaphilosophical lessons to draw from our empirical results.  We utilize scientometric tools and categorization analysis to provide an empirically reliable description of EIPM. Our methodological novelty lies in integrating the co-citation analysis tool with the conceptual resources from the philosoph...